FREE BOOKS

Author's List




PREV.   NEXT  
|<   220   221   222   223   224   225   226   227   228   229   230   231   232   233   234   235   236   237   238   239   240   241   242   243   244  
245   246   247   248   249   250   251   252   253   254   255   256   257   258   259   260   261   262   263   264   265   266   267   268   269   >>   >|  
." "A good brave dog defends his master." _Half credit._ "A dog defends his good master bravely." "A dog bravely defends his good master." "A good master bravely defends his dog." REMARKS. This is an excellent test. It involves no knowledge which may not be presupposed at the age in which it is given, and success therefore depends very little on experience. The worst that can be urged against it is that it may possibly be influenced to a certain extent by the amount of reading the subject has done. But this has not been demonstrated. At any rate, the test satisfies the most important requirement of a test of intelligence; namely, the percentage of successes increases rapidly and steadily from the lower to the higher levels of mental age. This experiment can be regarded as a variation of the completion test. Binet tells us, in fact, that it was directly suggested by the experiment of Ebbinghaus. As will readily be observed, however, it differs to a certain extent from the Ebbinghaus completion test. Ebbinghaus omits parts of a sentence and requires the subject to supply the omissions. In this test we give all the parts and require the formation of a sentence by rearrangement. The two experiments are psychologically similar in that they require the subject to relate given fragments into a meaningful whole. Success depends upon the ability of intelligence to utilize hints, or clues, and this in turn depends on the logical integrity of the associative processes. All but the highest grade of the feeble-minded fail with this test. This test is found in year XI of Binet's 1908 series and in year XII of his 1911 revision. Goddard and Kuhlmann retain it in the original location. That it is better placed in year XII is indicated by all the available statistics with normal children, except those of Goddard. With this exception, the results of various investigators for year XII are in remarkably close agreement, as the following figures will show:-- _Per cent passing at year XII_ Binet 66 Kuhlmann 68 Bobertag 78 Dougherty 64 Strong 72 Leviste and Morle 70 Stanford series (1911) 62 Stanford series (1913) 57 Stanford series (1914) 62 Princeton data 61 This agreement
PREV.   NEXT  
|<   220   221   222   223   224   225   226   227   228   229   230   231   232   233   234   235   236   237   238   239   240   241   242   243   244  
245   246   247   248   249   250   251   252   253   254   255   256   257   258   259   260   261   262   263   264   265   266   267   268   269   >>   >|  



Top keywords:

series

 

defends

 

master

 
bravely
 
Stanford
 

depends

 
Ebbinghaus
 

subject

 

intelligence

 

extent


completion
 

sentence

 

Goddard

 

Kuhlmann

 

require

 
experiment
 

agreement

 

feeble

 

minded

 
passing

highest

 
logical
 

ability

 

utilize

 

integrity

 

Princeton

 

associative

 
processes
 

revision

 

Leviste


exception

 

results

 

Dougherty

 

investigators

 

figures

 

remarkably

 

children

 

normal

 

original

 

location


Bobertag

 

retain

 

Strong

 

statistics

 

differs

 

demonstrated

 
reading
 

possibly

 

influenced

 

amount